Saint Trump and the Church of Birtherism: When Conspiracy Becomes Religion
Dayna
Wilson
Dr.
Noelle Phillips
English
1130 - 103
13
December 2020
Saint Trump and the Church of Birtherism: When
Conspiracy Becomes Religion
The
birtherism conspiracy theory, aiming to challenge the legitimacy of Barack
Obama’s presidential candidacy, was never a particularly novel or surprising
phenomenon. Rather, its inception and
subsequent circulation was the product of a culture that owes its nascence to
the oppression and undermining of Black Americans. The fact that Obama was both
the first Black presidential candidate and the first presidential candidate to be
accused of lacking the authority to run is no coincidence. The racism inherent in
the theory has been widely acknowledged and discussed, with a 2018 study demonstrating
clear correlation between racist bias and belief in birtherism (Jardina and
Traugott 60-80). The fervor with which proponents of the theory have sought to defend
it in the face of conclusive evidence disproving its validity, however, has
been less considered. Though Obama’s short form birth certificate was released
to the public in 2008, with the long form version following in 2011, a January
2016 poll reported that 34% of respondents continued to believe that Obama was
born outside of the United States (Morning Consult 5). This rejection of empirical evidence to cling
to a belief that cannot be substantiated draws parallels with how an individual
might validate their religious and spiritual beliefs. In seeking to fully
understand how the birther conspiracy theory became so widespread and difficult
to combat, one can benefit from examining the cognitive theories behind
religious adherence. When considering the relationship between organized religion
and birtherism, it is possible to develop an understanding of how the theory
came to pass, and as such, how the popularity of similar future theories might
be prevented.
One
cornerstone of most organized religions is the concept of “holy men” (and
occasionally women), institutionally elevated individuals who claim to speak
the divine truth, and who adherents of the faith view as divine representatives:
Catholic priests, Jewish rabbis, Islamic imams. Those who believe in
birtherism, meanwhile, have Donald Trump. Though Trump was not the originator
of the birtherist conspiracy theory, he is inarguably the most vocal proponent
of birtherism. Trump paved the way for his own presidential run by relying heavily
on the contentious nature of the theory to gain popularity with Republican voters
eager to discredit Obama (Serwer). Even when evidence mounted to disprove the
theory, Trump continued to argue for its validity, and followers continued to believe
him. This blind belief in the unsubstantiated claims of an authority figure is
consistent with the institutional cognition model of religion, which suggests
that “religious beliefs espoused by high-prestige exemplars should spread and evoke
more commitment than beliefs espoused by others” (Wood and Shaver 6).
Had
birtherism remained a low-profile theory, passed around between individuals, it
is unlikely that the same steadfast belief in it would have been witnessed.
Just as power is invested in religious figures due to their position within the
institution, birtherists afforded power to Donald Trump due to his wealth,
prestige, and visibility. He became a uniting figure in the movement, and as an
institution developed around the beliefs he espoused, he was afforded
institutional authority.
The establishment of Donald Trump as
a figurehead in the birtherist movement serves the further purpose of lending
guidance and order to its members, the desire for which has been identified as
integral in those who experience a high degree of religiosity. A 2000 study by
Steven Reiss identified 15 fundamental human desires, and the propensity for
highly religious individuals to experience each. Reiss found that those who
described themselves as extremely religious reported a much lower desire for
independence than those who were not religious, and as such tended to relish
being led by an external force (Reiss 50-51). It follows that a strong
correlation between belief in the unsubstantiated and a desire for external
leadership can be made. Donald Trump, a willing leader instilled with
institutional authority, serves to fulfill that apparent desire for leadership.
The result of this relationship between Trump and his followers is a
willingness to reject empirical evidence such as Obama’s short and long form
birth certificates. The birtherist’s faith in the word of Donald Trump
surpasses their willingness to accept demonstrable fact, just as a religious
adherent places all faith in a holy leader.
Though
Donald Trump’s position among believers is an important part of the religiosity
of birtherism, it is by no means the only parallel to be drawn. Psychologists
have long studied the cognitive thought processes behind the suspension of disbelief
in matters of religion, and one prevalent factor considered to be involved is the
human tendency towards confirmation bias. As Fiona Macdonald explains in an
article on ScienceAlert.com, individuals universally tend to seek out
source material that supports their belief systems, while rejecting those
sources that do not (Macdonald). An adherent might read one hundred articles
explaining that a statue of Jesus Christ appearing to weep blood is the result
of wax melting on a hot day, but will ignore them in favour of the one article
that claims that the phenomenon is a sign from God. Correspondingly, a birther can
find any number of articles enumerating the proofs that Barack Obama was born
on American soil yet choose to latch onto the few that claim without evidence
that he is foreign-born. Once the individual finds those few sources that
confirm irrational beliefs they might hold, these sources are valued above the
many that disprove the same beliefs.
Similarly. a phenomenon called minimally
counterintuitive (MCI) bias is considered to play a factor in the willingness
to view stories with glaring irrationalities, such as religious tenets or urban
legends, as indisputable fact. Stories that play into MCI bias “will balance a
minority of counterintuitive concepts with a majority of everyday, intuitive
events” (Stubberfield 90). In birther rhetoric, confirmable facts such as those
that Obama was born in Hawaii rather than on the American mainland, that his
father was African, and that he spent time living outside of America in his
youth have been conflated with counterintuitive inaccuracies, such as his
mother lying about his place of birth, or that he is secretly Muslim. Not only
are the irrationalities in the story more palatable when balanced with facts, scholars
have shown that the brain is actually more likely to latch onto the
counterintuitive aspects of a story and remember them above the factual aspects
(Stubberfeld 90).
The
cognitive similarities between those who subscribe to religious dogma and those
who believe in the legitimacy of the birther conspiracy are easily
identifiable, but the dilemma remains as to how one might apply understanding
of these processes towards preventing racist and harmful theories to achieve
legitimacy and widespread belief in the future. Given that Donald Trump can be
acknowledged to have lent a large amount of credibility to the movement, it
would be prudent to avoid allowing a media platform for such harmful and
inaccurate statements as those he has made. Without the frequent media
attention given to Trump’s spreading of the birther conspiracy, it would likely
have received less traction. Likewise, without an identifiable leader
continuously returning to the point and legitimizing it in the minds of the
public, it could have been forgotten along with any number of irrational
campaign accusations. Donald Trump’s involvement in the movement is an
extremely critical part of why it enjoyed the success it did.
As cognitive biases also can be
shown to play a substantial role in the development of dogmatic belief, ways to
confront them should also be examined in the prevention of further conspiracy
theories. While there is much speculation amongst psychologists on the best way
to do this, there is no identifiably agreed upon best course of action. In an
Economist interview, authors Nancy Rosenblum and Russell Muirhead argue in
favour of “fighting this fire with water: scrupulous recourse to argument and
evidence and explanations that are available to everyone and above all, subject
to correction” (N.C.), though like the religious adherent, proponents of
birtherism are unlikely to abandon their beliefs in the face of correction. It
seems that the preferable option is to avoid allowing such theories to develop
a quasi-religious following in the first place, again by avoiding media
attention and denying platforms to those who would claim harmful beliefs as
truth.
Religion serves an important purpose
in human existence, offering comfort and reassurance in the face of anxiety, a
sense of community, and an explanation for that which is beyond understanding. When
used to turn the community against a common enemy, however, it can become
dangerous and problematic, just as the birther conspiracy theory has been a steppingstone
to widespread racism and political unrest within the American public. Given the
similarities between religious thought processes and those that lend to belief
in conspiracy theories, much can be gained from the consideration of how
religious beliefs form and are supported by their adherents. With a better
understanding comes better ability to counter and prevent political arguments
and racist fallacies from becoming etched into the American conscience and
causing irreparable harm.
Works
Cited
Jardina, Ashley, and
Michael Traugott. “The Genesis of the Birther Rumor: Partisanship, Racial
Attitudes, and Political Knowledge.” The
Journal of Race, Ethnicity, and Politics, vol. 4, no. 1, 2019, pp. 60–80., doi:10.1017/rep.2018.25.
Macdonald, Fiona. “Researchers
Say They've Figured Out Why People Reject Science, And It's
Not
Ignorance.” Science Alert, 27 Jan. 2019, https://www.sciencealert.com/researchers-say-they-ve-figured-out-why-people-reject-science-and-it-s-not-ignorance.
Morning Consult. “National
Tracking Poll.” Questionnaire. 14-17 Jan. 2016. p. 5,
morningconsult.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/01/160102_topline_MCFull_v1_AP-2.pdf
N.C. “Conspiracy Theories
are Dangerous- Here’s How to Crush Them.” The Economist, 12
Aug.
2019, https://www.economist.com/open-future/2019/08/12/conspiracy-theories-are-dangerous-heres-how-to-crush-them.
Accessed 13 December, 2020
Reiss, Steven. “Why
People Turn to Religion: A Motivational Analysis.” Journal for the
Scientific
Study of Religion, vol. 39, no. 1, 2000, pp. 47–52. JSTOR,
www.jstor.org/stable/1387926. Accessed 14 Dec. 2020.
Serwer, Adam. “Birtherism
of a Nation.” The Atlantic, 13 May 2020,
https://www.theatlantic.com/ideas/archive/2020/05/birtherism-and-trump/610978/.
Accessed 13 December, 2020.
Stubbersfield, Joseph,
and Jamshid Tehrani. “Expect the Unexpected? Testing for Minimally
Counterintuitive
(MCI) Bias in the Transmission of Contemporary Legends: A Computational
Phylogenetic Approach.” Social Science Computer Review, vol. 31, no. 1, Feb.
2013, pp. 90–102, doi:10.1177/0894439312453567.
Wood, Connor, and John H.
Shaver. “Religion, Evolution, and the Basis of Institutions: The
Institutional
Cognition Model of Religion.” Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture,
vol. 2, no. 2, 2018, pp. 1–20. JSTOR, www.jstor.org/stable/10.26613/esic.2.2.89.
Comments
Post a Comment